Which psychologists focused on principles of perceptual organization




















These examples are instances of camouflage , the phenomenon in which objects are hidden from view but not by being occluded: instead, they are perceptually subdivided broken up internally and repartitioned, that is, their parts are grouped with parts of the surrounding environment. As used by animals in the struggle for survival and by humans in warfare, the power of Gestalt principles thus makes it possible for organisms and things which are in plain sight to become effectively invisible and therefore undetectable by adversaries.

Thus whether a physical object that is optically present exists or does not exist visually, depends on the interplay of perceptual laws. The pattern in Figure 6 a is readily partitioned into two components, a straight line and a wavy line that cross each other. This perceptual decomposition is strengthened by similarity Figure 6 b. An alternative decomposition of Figure 6 a into two abutting corners, depicted in Figure 6 c, does not seem to arise spontaneously; this can be explained by noting that it would violate the continuity principle.

However, an appeal to continuity does not explain why the partition in Figure 6 d does not spontaneously arise easily in Figure 6 a either, although both of its components are continuous lines. In another, related example, Figure 7 a spontaneously decomposes into a semi-wheel with curved cogs touching a rectangular 'snake'.

However, this perceptual outcome actually violates the continuity principle, because at the point at which the two components touch, this decomposition involves angles, instead of following the directions of the crossing continuous lines. An even clearer decomposition is achieved by introducing similarity as well Figure 7 b.

However, similarity can also be used to enhance a radically different decomposition into two crossing twisted threads, favored by continuity, as indicated in Figure 7 c. According to the Gestalt viewpoint, the dominant percepts in Figure 6 a and Figure 7 a are instances of the good Gestalt principle : elements tend to be grouped together if they are parts of a pattern which is a good Gestalt, meaning as simple, orderly, balanced, unified, coherent, regular, etc as possible, given the input.

In this sense, the straight line and the wavy line perceived in Figure 6 a are better forms than the pairs of lines in Figure 6 c and Figure 6 d, and in Figure 7 a the cog wheel and the snake are better forms than the hybrid shapes in Figure 7 c, that would be generated in Figure 7 a by conforming to the continuity principle at the crossing point.

In such cases global regularity takes precedence over local relations. In some cases the visual input is organized according to the past experience principle : elements tend to be grouped together if they were together often in the past experience of the observer. For example, we tend to perceive the pattern in Figure 8 a as a meaningful word, built up from strokes which are grouped to form particular letters of the Roman alphabet such as 'm', 'i', 'n', etc.

Note that the individual letters are rather clearly and distinctly perceived as 'natural' parts of the connected figure, and are only slightly easier to discern and discriminate if further individuated through separation Figure 8 b or coloration Figure 8 c. However, in addition to this standard segmentation into letters, the pattern Figure 8 a has many other alternate partitions, such as the one demonstrated through separation and coloration in Figure 8 d and Figure 8 e.

But, in contrast to the standard segmentation, discerning and discriminating these alternate components some of which are 'non-letters' within Figure 8 a is a cumbersome task, similar to the laborious search for the hidden shape in Figures 6c-e; furthermore, the standard segmentation is to some extent perceivable even in Figure 8 e, where it competes with the segmentation based on the similarity principle.

The spontaneity and ease of the standard, dominantly perceived organization of the strokes into letters, is plausibly mainly due to past experience, that is, to our familiarity with words as written in the script form of the Roman alphabet. This particular organization might not occur for observers lacking such familiarity; furthermore, the alternate partition would presumably be natural for observers used to an alphabet whose letters would correspond to the sub-wholes in Figure 8 d and Figure 8 e.

Note also that in print perhaps the most potent Gestalt principle is proximity: simply inserting larger blank spaces between words than between letters a device not used in antiquity helps group together the letters correctly, and establish words as the salient visual units in the text.

The importance of blank spaces is demonstrated by the difficulty wehavewhenreadingtextnotseparatedbyblanks an dev enmor ew henbl an kspa cesap pea rinwr ongpl aces. Although acknowledged by the gestaltists, the experience-based principle was deemed of secondary importance, compared with the other, stimulus-based principles, and easily dominated by them. As an example, in the pattern in Figure 8 f, in which a slightly overlapping inverted version is added, the original stimulus is much harder to see, due to the appearance of numerous new salient sub-patterns, generated by continuity and closure.

The acoustic input is just a one-dimensional temporally varying air pressure waveform, but based on it we can perceive an auditory scene involving multiple sources of human speech, vocal and instrumental music, animal sounds and other nature noises, occasionally all occurring at the same time, each with its own sub-phrasing and structure. Some visual Gestalt principles directly apply in the acoustic domain, but mainly in a temporal rather than spatial form.

For example, silence or background noise, interrupted by a loud sound, followed again by silence or noise, is an auditory analogue of a figure on a ground. Similarly, a regular series of identical short clicks is an analogue of Figure 2 a, with equal temporal intervals between sound events playing the role of equal spatial distances.

However, such a phenomenal segmentation is achieved much more naturally and easily by simply increasing the intervals between some clicks, analogously to Figure 2 b. Auditory analogues of instances of the visual similarity principle, as illustrated in Figure 3 , are also readily established, but with differences and similarities of color, size etc being replaced by differences and similarities of loudness, pitch, and timbre of sounds.

Auditory analogues of some other Gestalt principles may also be constructed. The principles described above, together with others not illustrated here, such as the symmetry principle symmetrical components will tend to group together , the convexity principle convex rather than concave patterns will tend to be perceived as figures , and others, are part of the classical heritage of perception studies.

In contemporary research, of which only a few examples will be noted below, the seminal insights and issues raised by the gestaltists are developed and extended in various directions.

In this article, I want to share seven gestalt laws and show how to apply them in practice. Elements that have similar visual appearance seems to be more related. In the image below, you probably see the groupings of colored circles as rows rather than just a collection of individual circles. Elements can visually be grouped together if they have visual similarities. Our mind likes symmetrical objects because symmetry creates an impression of stability and order.

When you arrange UI Elements to make them symmetrical to each other, you simplify the process scanning for users. For example, a symmetrical navigation menu tends to be perceived as more stable than the asymmetrical one.

Quick note: Symmetrical layouts can look boring. Thus, you might want to introduce some asymmetry in design to make it more dynamic.

Elements tend to be perceived into groups if they are sharing an area with a clearly defined boundary. By adding borders around an element or a group of items , you separate it from surrounding elements. Check the cards in the image below. Subtle shadow and clearly visible borders create an impression of individual objects. We know which elements are placed in the foreground and with ones are in the background intuitively.

The law of similarity states that similar things tend to appear grouped together. Grouping can occur in both visual and auditory stimuli. In the image at the top of this page, for example, you probably see two separate groupings of colored circles as rows rather than just a collection of dots. This law holds that when you're presented with a set of ambiguous or complex objects, your brain will make them appear as simple as possible.

For example, when presented with the Olympic logo, you see overlapping circles rather than an assortment of curved, connected lines. According to the law of proximity, things that are close together seem more related than things that are spaced farther apart.

In the image at the top of the page, the circles on the left appear to be part of one grouping while those on the right appear to be part of another. Because the objects are close to each other, we group them together. The law of continuity holds that points that are connected by straight or curving lines are seen in a way that follows the smoothest path.

In other words, elements in a line or curve seem more related to one another than those positioned randomly. According to the law of closure, we perceive elements as belonging to the same group if they seem to complete some entity.

Our brains often ignore contradictory information and fill in gaps in information. In the image at the top of the page, you probably see the shape of a diamond because your brain fills in the missing gaps in order to create a meaningful image. The Gestalt law of common region says that when elements are located in the same closed region, we perceive them as belonging to the same group. Look at the last image at the top of the page. The circles are right next to each other so that the dot at the end of one circle is actually closer to the dot at the end of the neighboring circle.

But despite how close those two dots are, we see the dots inside the circles as belonging together. Creating a clearly defined boundary can overpower other Gestalt laws such as the law of proximity.

The Gestalt laws of perceptual organization present a set of principles for understanding some of the ways in which perception works. Research continues to offer insights into perception and how we see the world.

These principles of organization play a role in perception, but it is also important to remember that they can sometimes lead to incorrect perceptions of the world. It is important to remember that while these principles are referred to as laws of perceptual organization, they are actually heuristics or shortcuts. Proximity 4. Similarity 5. Continuity 6. Gestalt psychologists claimed that when we receive sensations that form an incomplete or unfinished visual image or sound, we tend to overlook the incompleteness and perceive the image or sound as a complete or finished unit.

This tendency to fill in the gaps is referred to as closure. Show figure 7. The partial outlines of the figure will be filled out and your friend might say that it is a square, though it is not, in-fact, one. But even if it is seen as an incomplete square it shows that your friend first saw a square and later registered its incompleteness. This illustrates that the principle of closure was in operation. This shows a tendency to perceive meaningful objects.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000